Research Papers On Human Elephant Conflict Pdf To Word

1. Karanth KK, Gopalaswamy AM, Prasad PK, Dasgupta S. Patterns of human–wildlife conflicts and compensation: Insights from Western Ghats protected areas. Biol Conserv. 2013;166:175–85.

2. Nyhus P, Tilson R. Agroforestry, elephants, and tigers: balancing conservation theory and practice in human-dominated landscapes of Southeast Asia. Agriculture, ecosystems & environment. 2004;104(1):87–97.

3. Zhang L, Ma L, Feng L. New challenges facing traditional nature reserves: Asian elephant (Elephas maximus) conservation in China. Integrative Zoology. 2006;1(4):179–87. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-4877.2006.00031.x[PubMed]

4. Zomer RJ, Trabucco A, Wang M, Lang R, Chen H, Metzger MJ, et al. Environmental stratification to model climate change impacts on biodiversity and rubber production in Xishuangbanna, Yunnan, China. Biol Conserv. 2014;170:264–73.

5. IUCN. The IUCN Red List of Threatened Species 2014 [updated 2014.3; cited 2014 10 November]. Available from: www.iucnredlist.org.

6. China Ministry of Agriculture, State Forestry Administration. National key protected wildlife directory. The state council bulletin of the People's Republic of China. 2003;23.

7. Sun G, Xu Q, Jin K, Wang Z, Lang Y. The historical withdrawal of wild Elephas maximus in China and its relationship with human population pressure. Journal of Northeast Forestry University. 1997;26(4):47–50.

8. Zhang L. Current status and research progress of Asian Elephant in China. The Biological Bulletin. 2006;42(11):1–2.

9. Zhang L. Current conservation status and research progress on Asian elephants in China. Gajah. 2007:35.

10. Sukumar R. The living elephants: evolutionary ecology, behaviour, and conservation: Oxford University Press; 2003.

11. He Q, Wu Z, Zhou W, Dong R. Perception and attitudes of local communities towards wild elephant-related problems and conservation in Xishuangbanna, southwestern China. Chin Geogr Sci. 2011;21(5):629–36. doi: 10.1007/s11769-011-0499-4

12. Xu J, Fox J, Vogler JB, Yongshou ZPF, Lixin Y, Jie Q, et al. Land-use and land-cover change and farmer vulnerability in Xishuangbanna prefecture in southwestern China. Environ Manage. 2005;36(3):404–13. [PubMed]

13. Xu J, Grumbine RE, Beckschäfer P. Landscape transformation through the use of ecological and socioeconomic indicators in Xishuangbanna, Southwest China, Mekong Region. Ecological Indicators. 2014;36(0):749–56. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ecolind.2012.08.023.

14. XSBN-NNR N. Master plan of Xishuangbanna National Nature reserve Yunnan: XSBN NNR, 2010.

15. Li Z, Ma Y, Li H, Peng M, Liu W. Relation of land use and cover change to topography in Xishuangbanna, Southwest China. Journal of Plant Ecology (Chinese Version). 2008;32(5):1091–103.

16. Chen S, Yi Z-F, Campos-Arceiz A, Chen M-Y, Webb EL. Developing a spatially-explicit, sustainable and risk-based insurance scheme to mitigate human–wildlife conflict. Biol Conserv. 2013;168:31–9.

17. Graham MD, Notter B, Adams WM, Lee PC, Ochieng TN. Patterns of crop-raiding by elephants, Loxodonta africana, in Laikipia, Kenya, and the management of human–elephant conflict. Systematics and Biodiversity. 2010;8(4):435–45.

18. Hoare R. African elephants and humans in conflict: the outlook for co‐existence. Oryx. 2000;34(1):34–8.

19. Sitati N, Walpole M, Smith R, Leader‐Williams N. Predicting spatial aspects of human–elephant conflict. J Appl Ecol. 2003;40(4):667–77.

20. Chartier L, Zimmermann A, Ladle RJ. Habitat loss and human–elephant conflict in Assam, India: does a critical threshold exist?Oryx. 2011;45(4):528–33.

21. Das JP, Lahkar BP, Talukdar BK. Increasing trend of human elephant conflict in Golaghat District, Assam, India: issues and concerns. Gajah. 2012:34.

22. Gubbi S. Patterns and correlates of human–elephant conflict around a south Indian reserve. Biological Conservation. 2012;148(1):88–95.

23. Wilson S, Davies TE, Hazarika N, Zimmermann A. Understanding spatial and temporal patterns of human–elephant conflict in Assam, India. Oryx. 2013:1–10.

24. Goswami VR, Medhi K, Nichols JD, Oli MK. Mechanistic understanding of human–wildlife conflict through a novel application of dynamic occupancy models. Conservation Biology. 2015;29(4):1100–10. doi: 10.1111/cobi.12475[PubMed]

25. Lahkar B, Das J, Nath N, Dey S, Brahma N, Sarma P. A study of habitat utilization patterns of Asian elephant Elephas maximus and current status of human elephant conflict in Manas National Park within Chirang-Ripu Elephant Reserve, Assam. Report, Aaranyak, Guwahati, Assam, India. 2007.

26. Liu J, Ouyang Z, Miao H. Environmental attitudes of stakeholders and their perceptions regarding protected area-community conflicts: A case study in China. Journal of Environmental Management. 2010;91(11):2254–62. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvman.2010.06.007[PubMed]

27. Cai J, Jiang Z, Zeng Y, Li C, Bravery BD. Factors affecting crop damage by wild boar and methods of mitigation in a giant panda reserve. Eur J Wildlife Res. 2008;54(4):723–8.

28. Yang W, He S, Shen Y. Spatiotemporal Patterns of Wildlife Damages in Southern Part of Mt. Baima-Xueshan. Journal of Mountain Science. 2009;3:009.

29. Myers N, Mittermeier RA, Mittermeier CG, Da Fonseca GA, Kent J. Biodiversity hotspots for conservation priorities. Nature. 2000;403(6772):853–8. [PubMed]

30. Zhang J, Cao M. Tropical forest vegetation of Xishuangbanna, SW China and its secondary changes, with special reference to some problems in local nature conservation. Biol Conserv. 1995;73(3):229–38.

31. MacKinnon. Ecologival guidelines for the development of Xishuangbanna Prefecture, Yunnan Province, China Gland Switzerland: WWF, 1987.

32. Okabe A, Satoh T, Sugihara K. A kernel density estimation method for networks, its computational method and a GIS-based tool. International Journal of Geographical Information Science. 2009;23(1):7–32. doi: 10.1080/13658810802475491

33. Wilson R. Free GIS Data 2014 [cited 2014 24th March]. Available from: http://freegisdata.rtwilson.com/.

34. USGS. Shuttle Radar Topography Mission, Arc Second scene SRTM_ASTGTM2 College Park, Maryland: Global Land Cover Facility, University of Maryland; 2013 [updated February 2000; cited 2013]. Available from: http://glcf.umd.edu/data/srtm/.

35. Tsinghua U. Finer resolution observation and monitoring-global land cover 2013 [cited 2013 20th March 2013]. Available from: http://data.ess.tsinghua.edu.cn/.

36. Senf C, Pflugmacher D, van der Linden S, Hostert P. Mapping Rubber Plantations and Natural Forests in Xishuangbanna (Southwest China) Using Multi-Spectral Phenological Metrics from MODIS Time Series. Remote Sensing. 2013;5(6):2795–812.

37. Hollander M, Wolfe D. Nonparametric Statistical Methods. John Wiley and Sons, New York; 1973. p. 185–94.

38. Zuur A, Ieno EN, Walker N, Saveliev AA, Smith GM. Mixed effects models and extensions in ecology with R: Springer Science & Business Media; 2009.

39. Zeileis A, Kleiber C, Jackman S. Regression Models for Count Data in R. Journal of Statistical Software. 2007;27(8):1–25.

40. Jackman S, Zeileis A. pscl: Political Science Computational Laboratory, Stanford University. 2015.

41. Guerbois C, Chapanda E, Fritz H. Combining multi‐scale socio‐ecological approaches to understand the susceptibility of subsistence farmers to elephant crop raiding on the edge of a protected area. Journal of Applied Ecology. 2012;49(5):1149–58.

42. Songhurst A, Coulson T. Exploring the effects of spatial autocorrelation when identifying key drivers of wildlife crop-raiding. Ecology & Evolution. 2014;4(5):582–93. [PMC free article][PubMed]

43. Dormann C F., McPherson J M., Araújo M B., Bivand R, Bolliger J, Carl G, et al. Methods to account for spatial autocorrelation in the analysis of species distributional data: a review. Ecography. 2007;30(5):609–28. doi: 10.1111/j.2007.0906–7590.05171.x

44. Lacoeuilhe A, Machon N, Julien J-F, Kerbiriou C. Effects of hedgerows on bats and bush crickets at different spatial scales. Acta Oecologica. 2016;71:61–72. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.actao.2016.01.009.

45. Bivand R, Hauke J, Kossowski T. Computing the Jacobian in Gaussian Spatial Autoregressive Models: An Illustrated Comparison of Available Methods. Geographical Analysis. 2013;45(2):150–79.

46. Bivand R, Piras G. Comparing Implementations of Estimation Methods for Spatial Econometrics. Journal of Statistical Software. 2015;63(18):1–36.

47. R Development Core Team R. R: A language and environment for statistical computing Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing,; 2014. Available from: http://www.R-project.org/.

48. Sakamoto Y, Ishiguro M, Kitagawa G. Akaike information criterion statistics Dordrecht, The Netherlands: D Reidel; 1986.

49. Burnham KP, Anderson DR. Model selection and multimodel inference: a practical information-theoretic approach: Springer; 2002.

50. Carpenter J, Bithell J. Bootstrap con" dence intervals: when, which, what?A practical guide for medical statisticians. 2000. [PubMed]

51. Fawcett T. An introduction to ROC analysis. Pattern Recognition Letters. 2006;27(8):861–74.

52. Nagelkerke NJ. A note on a general definition of the coefficient of determination. Biometrika. 1991;78(3):691–2.

53. Quantum GIS Development Team Q. Quantum GIS Geographic Information System. Open Source Geospatial Foundation Project. 2013. Available from: http://qgis.osgeo.org.

54. Joshi R, Singh R. Feeding behaviour of wild Asian Elephants (Elephas maximus) in the Rajaji National Park. Journal of American Science. 2008;4(2):34–48.

55. Webber CE, Sereivathana T, Maltby MP, Lee PC. Elephant crop-raiding and human–elephant conflict in Cambodia: crop selection and seasonal timings of raids. Oryx. 2011;45(02):243–51. doi: 10.1017/S0030605310000335

56. Chen J, Deng X, Zhang L, Bai Z. Diet composition and foraging ecology of Asian elephants in Shangyong, Xishuangbanna, China. Acta Ecologica Sinica. 2006;26(2):209–316.

57. Yang Z, Chen M, Dong Y, Liu L, Yang S. Analysis on Asian Elephants' Habitat Situation in Menyang Sub- reserve of Xishuanbanna National Nature Reserve. Forest Inventory and Planning. 2006;31(3):49–51.

58. Wu Jinliang JW, Jiansheng Hu, Zongqiang Li, Chunhong Li. Distribution dynamic of Asian Elephant in Xishuangbanna in the past 40 years. Chinese Wildlife. 1999;20(3):8–9.

59. Lin L, Zhang L, Luo A, Wang L, Zhang L. Population dynamics, structure and seasonal distribution pattern of Asian elephant (Elephas maximus) in Shangyong Protected Area, Yunnan, China. Acta Theriologica Sinica. 2011;31(3):226–34.

60. Yuan Z, Zhang L. Population and activity characteristic of wild Asian elephants in the Wild Elephant Valley, Xishuangbanna National Nature Reserve, Yunnan, China. Acta Theriologica Sinica. 2006;26(4):359–67.

61. Zong J, Liu S, Xu H, Wang L, Guo X. Population size and distribution changes of Asian elephant in Menglazi Nature Reserve, Xishuangbanna Nature Reserve. Forest Invenory and Planning. 2014;39(1):89–93.

62. Yi Z-F, Cannon CH, Chen J, Ye C-X, Swetnam RD. Developing indicators of economic value and biodiversity loss for rubber plantations in Xishuangbanna, southwest China: A case study from Menglun township. Ecological Indicators. 2013.

Rearticulating the myth of human–wildlife conflict

Authors

  • M. Nils Peterson,

    1. Fisheries and Wildlife Sciences Program, Department of Forestry & Environmental Resources, North Carolina State University, Raleigh, NC 27695-7646, USA
    Search for more papers by this author
  • Jessie L. Birckhead,

    1. Fisheries and Wildlife Sciences Program, Department of Forestry & Environmental Resources, North Carolina State University, Raleigh, NC 27695-7646, USA
    Search for more papers by this author
  • Kirsten Leong,

    1. Human Dimensions of Biological Resource Management Biological Resource Management Division, National Park Service, Fort Collins, CO, USA
    Search for more papers by this author
  • Markus J. Peterson,

    1. Department of Wildlife & Fisheries Sciences, Texas A&M University, College Station, TX, USA
    Search for more papers by this author
  • Tarla Rai Peterson

    1. Department of Wildlife & Fisheries Sciences, Texas A&M University, College Station, TX, USA
    2. Department of Urban and Rural Development, Swedish University of Agricultural Sciences, Uppsala 75007, Sweden
    Search for more papers by this author

Correspondence
Markus Nils Peterson, North Carolina State University, Box 7646, Raleigh, NC 27695-7646, USA. Tel: 919-515-7588; fax: 919-515-5110. E-mail: nils_peterson@ncsu.edu

Abstract

Human–wildlife conflict has emerged as the central vocabulary for cases requiring balance between resource demands of humans and wildlife. This phrase is problematic because, given traditional definitions of conflict, it positions wildlife as conscious human antagonists. We used content analysis of wildlife conservation publications and professional meeting presentations to explore the use of the phrase, human–wildlife conflict, and compared competing models explaining its usage. Of the 422 publications and presentations using human–wildlife conflict, only 1 reflected a traditional definition of conflict, >95% referred to reports of animal damage to entities human care about, and <4% referred to human–human conflict. Usage of human–wildlife conflict was related to species type (herbivores with human food, carnivores with human safety, meso-mammals with property), development level of the nation where the study occurred (less developed nations with human food and more developed nations with human safety and property damage), and whether the study occurred on private lands or protected areas (protected areas with human–human conflict and other areas with property damage). We argue that the phrase, human–wildlife conflict, is detrimental to coexistence between humans and wildlife, and suggest comic reframing to facilitate a more productive interpretation of human–wildlife relationships.

Introduction

Understanding the changing social contexts for conflict between conservation and human welfare is central to biodiversity conservation (Czech et al. 1998; Chan et al. 2007; Peterson et al. 2008). Conflicts associated with biodiversity conservation reflect material as well as socially constructed realities (Guyer & Richards 1996; Schlosser 2006). Considerable research documents the material conditions where promoting human welfare while simultaneously conserving biodiversity appear incompatible (Kerr & Currie 1995; Forester & Machlis 1996; Czech et al. 2000), but little research addresses how social constructions of those material conditions shape biodiversity conservation (Herda-Rapp & Goedeke 2005; Chan et al. 2007; Leong 2010). Scientists who hope to influence conservation policy must understand those constructions (Ludwig 2001).

Although all human experience is grounded in material reality, materiality alone is insufficient to motivate social action (Burke [1950]1969; Peterson 1997). People's past experiences, beliefs, and values frame their perceptions (Dingwall 2002; Lewicki et al. 2003). These frames influence what is important and shape people's interpretation of material reality. One practical way to identify and even shape future frames is to examine how they are instantiated through language. In this study, we focused on rhetorical framing, or language used to provoke identification and cooperation among humans. Rhetoric influences human motivation and action by shaping vocabularies. These vocabularies form terministic screens, wherein individual words (terms) interact to emphasize some aspects of reality, while deemphasizing others. The terministic screens people use enable them to consider and discuss the importance, meaning, and demands (action called for) of their experience. Terministic screens, therefore, direct attention by emphasizing or deemphasizing different elements of reality. As we communicate, human perceptions become implications of the terministic screen through which they are articulated (Burke 1966). Terministic screens shape the way society reacts to environmental challenges by constraining possibilities. For example, in the United States, the jobs versus environment terministic screen pitted endangered species conservation against human welfare during the Reagan and first Bush administrations (Goodstein 1999; Peterson et al. 2004). Changing that terministic screen to environmental protection as precondition for healthy economy (fulfilled through approaches such as habitat conservation plans) during the Clinton administration encouraged simultaneous promotion of both endangered species conservation and job growth. These approaches allowed individuals, communities, and companies to harm endangered species if they promised sufficient mitigation to offset the damage. Although the conservation benefits of this terministic shift are debatable, a major shift occurred nonetheless.

Terministic screens become problematic in biodiversity conservation contexts when they frame the needs of humans and wildlife as arising from conscious antagonism. The terministic screen formed by the phrase human–wildlife conflict represents such a case because it emphasizes conscious antagonism between wildlife and humans (Conover 2002; Graham et al. 2005). Cases where resource demands of humans and wildlife must be balanced could be described as human–wildlife coexistence (Madden 2004), human–wildlife competition (Matthiopoulos et al. 2008), or human–human conflict (Marshall et al. 2007). The human–wildlife conflict terministic screen, however, is used to frame some of the most high-profile wildlife conservation cases in the world (e.g., whales, seals, sea turtles, tigers, wolves, bears, elephants; Sukumar 1991; Dublin & Hoare 2004; Omondi et al. 2004; Patterson et al. 2004). Concepts encompassed by the phrase, human–wildlife conflict, have inspired books and international collaborations, started centers, and generally shaped research addressing interactions between humans and wildlife. The Jack H. Berryman Institute for Wildlife Damage Management, for example, changed its web tagline from “Wildlife Damage Management” to “Resolving Human–Wildlife Conflicts” in 2007. This change coincided with the inaugural issue of its academic journal, Human–Wildlife Conflicts.

Conflict is a well developed, if interdisciplinary, concept. Definitions generally converge around “expressed disagreements among people who see incompatible goals and potential interference in achieving these goals” (Pearce & Littlejohn 1997; Peterson et al. 2002:947; Pruitt & Kim 2004). Environmental conflicts implicate consciousness and social interaction, are intensely political, and are always linked to power relationships and values (Peterson & Franks 2005; Raik et al. 2008). Recent definitions of biodiversity conflict reflect these well-documented dimensions of conflict in part by suggesting that interests of two or more parties must clash and at least one of the parties must attempt to assert its interests at the expense of the other (Bennett et al. 2001; Marshall et al. 2007). This definition captures the essential idea that conflict emerges out of actors’ interpretation of a situation instead of simply competition for limited resources (Peterson et al. 2002; Yasmi et al. 2006). This version of conflict excludes most wildlife species as parties to conflict because few if any wild species could be construed as being simultaneously aware of their own goals, aware of human goals, and purposefully seeking to undermine human goal-seeking capacity.

In this article we use content analysis of journal articles, reports, books, and conference presentations drawn from conservation biology venues to analyze how the phrase, human–wildlife conflict frames perceptions of interactions between humans and wildlife. We then determine how the species involved and study area locations relate to those constructions. We conclude by suggesting ways conservation biologists can address challenges associated with human–wildlife coexistence without sacrificing the rhetorically powerful language of conflict.

Methods

We used a grounded theory approach to guide content analysis because we were attempting to generate theory from data (Corbin & Strauss 2008). We chose to investigate the use and meaning of human–wildlife conflict because of its mixed usage in articles we read and proposals we reviewed. We sensed an inconsistency in the usage of human–wildlife conflict that ranged from descriptions of people being afraid of wildlife (Kaltenborn et al. 2006) to attacks and counter-attacks between wildlife and people (Jackson & Wangchuk 2004; Bagchi & Mishra 2006). We assumed the most carefully designed and well-described studies addressing human–wildlife conflict would be found in published literature, and that studies most indicative of future trends would be found in papers from professional meetings related to wildlife conservation. Content analysis began with a review of 10 published journal articles to ensure that abstracts were sufficiently representative of the papers to allow the use of abstracts as the source of material for analysis. Although some articles used multiple interpretations of human–wildlife conflict, none contradicted the primary interpretation identified in the abstract.

We then searched for journal articles, books, and reports (hereafter publications) that used combinations of the words “human”, “wildlife”, and “conflict” in their titles, key words, and/or abstracts using Web of Science, Wildlife and Ecology Studies Worldwide, and Google Scholar (November 2008). This search yielded 1,348 publications. We limited our analysis to 306 of these publications that used the phrase, human–wildlife conflict, or a species-specific alternative (e.g., human–bear conflict). We used 1983 as a cutoff point given the paucity of older human–wildlife conflict publications. We also searched available conference presentation abstracts from 2004 to 2008 for The Society for Conservation Biology (2005–2008), The Wildlife Society (2004–2008), and the Human Dimensions of Wildlife conference (2008). We analyzed the conference presentation abstracts for the same key words as the publications. A total of 117 conference presentation abstracts used the phrase, human–wildlife conflict, or a species-specific version of the phrase.

J. Birckhead and N. Peterson used thematic content analysis to develop categories for human–wildlife conflict (Peterson et al. 1994; Strauss & Corbin 1997). The content analysis entailed analyzing the titles and abstracts word-by-word to develop provisional concepts for how human–wildlife conflict was used. We reached saturation (no new categories emerged) after developing 15 categories. These categories then were collapsed into nine (seven types of animal damage, human–human conflict, and actual human–wildlife conflict; Table 1). During this process, we used the constant comparison technique to raise questions regarding categories, and used memos to document and analyze ideas about categories as they emerged during thematic analysis (Corbin & Strauss 2008). The development status of nations, whether or not the study was conducted in or directly adjacent to a protected area, and the types of species associated with studies emerged as categories explaining the use of the term, human–wildlife conflict. Species categories ultimately included two types of charismatic megafauna (herbivores and carnivores), meso-mammals (e.g., Castor canadensis, Trichosurus vulpecula), and birds.

Animal damage–crops7624.82925.0
Animal damage–livestock7624.82622.2
Animal damage–human safety4213.72219.0
Animal damage–property damage3110.11916.4
Animal damage–fisheries17 5.6 2 1.7
Human–human conflict11 3.6 1 0.9
Animal damage–transportation10 3.3 5 4.3
Animal damage–gardens 5 1.6 00
Human–wildlife conflict 1 0.3 00
Review3712.11210.3

As with most grounded theory research, our results and methods were intertwined (Strauss & Corbin 1997). Concepts of human–wildlife conflict usage that emerged during thematic analysis were used to generate definitions for each category that could be used to train coders and assess intercoder reliability (Krippendorff 2004). To assess reliability of the thematic analysis, an undergraduate worker recoded all the publications and presentations. Intercoder reliability was high for coding the types of human–wildlife conflict in publications (κ= 0.941, P < 0.001, n= 306) and conference presentation abstracts (κ= 0.894, P < 0.001, n= 117). We used the three categories obtained through thematic analysis (species type, development level of the nation where the study occurred, and whether the study occurred in a protected area) to formulate competing nominal logistic regression models (JMP 8.0.1, SAS Institute, Cary, NC, USA) that could account for four categories considered human–wildlife conflict in both publications and presentations (Table 2). We compared these models within an information-theoretic approach to model selection (Burnham & Anderson 2002). Development level of the nation where each study occurred was quantified using the Human Development Index value (0 to 1) for each nation as reported in the 2009 United Nations Development Programme Report (http://hdr.undp.org/en/statistics/). Some papers (n= 37) and presentations (n= 22) were conducted at a continental or global scale and were not assigned a Human Development Index value. We developed competing models to explain usage of human–wildlife conflict using each of the three variables singly, in pairs, collectively, and with interactions. We included three interactions: (1) species type and Human Development Index value, (2) species type and whether the study occurred in a protected area, and (3) Human Development Index value and whether the study occurred in a protected area. We used Akaike's information criterion corrected for small sample size (AICc) to compare models and calculated the associated Akaike weights for each model [wi; the probability that the current (ith) model is the best-approximating model among those considered].

Publications (n)(47)(104)(26)(34)(27)(113)(123)(81)(146)
 Food83.068.323.167.685.277.550.067.962.8
 Safety 8.526.911.55.97.417.119.421.817.5
 Property 6.4 2.961.517.6 3.7 0.926.9 2.618.2
 Human–human 2.1 1.9 3.8 8.8 3.7 4.5 3.7 7.7 1.5
Presentations (n)(21)(41)(7)(1)(31)(0)(42)(24)(50)
 Food58.343.90077.4NA21.454.240.0
 Safety16.736.628.6019.4NA38.129.230.0
 Property 8.319.571.4100 3.2NA38.116.728.0
 Human–human 8.30000NA2.402.0

Results

Human–wildlife conflict usage in publications (journal articles [n= 277, 90.5%], books [n= 16, 5.2%], reports [n= 13, 4.2%]) typically referred to animal damage (Table 1). Of all publications and conference presentations using the term, human–wildlife conflict, we coded one as actual human–wildlife conflict (Warne & Jones 2003). In that paper, magpies (Cracticus tibicen) repeatedly attacked specific humans that were seen as threats, and humans retaliated. The authors found “all intruding cyclists and mail deliverers were attacked, magpies did not attack all pedestrian intruders, suggesting possible discrimination within this category” (Warne & Jones 2003:265). In conference presentation abstracts, human–wildlife conflict was typically used to describe animal damage (Table 1).

When human–wildlife conflict usage referred to wildlife damaging consuming crops or livestock, we coded the document as animal damage–crops and animal damage–livestock, respectively (Table 1). These categories combined damages suffered by both subsistence and commercial producers, and included indirect effects where wildlife consumed a resource (e.g., water) used in crop or livestock production. “Human–elephant conflict, in particular the damage caused by elephants to smallholder crops, is a major challenge to the conservation of African elephant” (Graham & Ochieng 2008:76) represents expressions of human–wildlife conflict coded as animal damage–crops. “Therefore, wolf–human conflicts are common, chiefly because of wolf depredation on livestock” (Kumar & Rahmani 1997:466) represents animal damage–livestock.

When authors referred to wildlife consuming fish that humans wanted to use in aquaculture or in open water settings (e.g., commercial, subsistence, and recreational fishing) as human–wildlife conflict, we coded it animal damage–fisheries (Table 1). “Conflict between humans and cormorants occur because of depredation of fish from nets and weirs, consumption of trout and migrating salmon smelts” (Milton et al. 1995:91) represents articles coded as animal damage–fisheries. Wildlife damage to crops, livestock, and fisheries were grouped as a food-related category for quantitative analysis (Table 2).

When human–wildlife conflict usage alluded to wildlife frightening, injuring, killing, or transmitting infectious agents to humans, we coded the document animal damage–human safety (Tables 1 and 2). “When humans enter sloth bear habitat or sloth bears enter crop fields, conflicts occur that cause numerous human casualties” (Rajpurohit & Krausman 2000:393) typifies usage of human–wildlife conflict coded as animal damage–human safety.

When authors referred to wildlife damaging nonagricultural property as human–wildlife conflict we coded it as animal damage–property damage (Table 1). This category primarily included wildlife nesting on buildings and/or damaging physical structures. “A simpler way to limit bat–human conflicts may be to modify new and existing buildings to discourage colonies from initially taking up residence” (Williams & Brittingham 1997:359) typifies usage of human–wildlife conflict coded animal damage–property damage. We coded usage of human–wildlife conflict alluding to wildlife damaging vehicles (e.g., airplanes) or interfering with their use as animal damage–transportation (Table 1). “These concurrent activities have resulted in increasing human–wildlife conflicts, most visibly deer–vehicle collisions” (Sullivan & Messmer 2003:163) typifies usage of human–wildlife conflict coded animal damage–transportation. When human–wildlife conflict usage alluded to wildlife raiding gardens that were not used by humans for subsistence purposes, we coded the document as animal damage–gardens (Table 1). “Several human–wildlife conflicts were identified … bandicoots were disliked by a small minority (3%) of residents owing to the holes they dig in lawns and gardens” (FitzGibbon & Jones 2006:233) typifies usage of human–wildlife conflict coded animal damage–gardens. The property, transportation, and garden categories were grouped in a property category for quantitative analysis (Table 2).

When human–wildlife conflict referred to human disagreements over wildlife management decisions (e.g., preferred responses to conservation mandates), we coded the document as human–human conflict (Tables 1 and 2). “The growing popularity of caving and closing down caves for bat conservation has brought about human–wildlife conflicts” (Thorne 1990) typified human–wildlife conflict usage we coded human–human conflict. Finally, we created a review category for documents that discussed multiple interpretations of human–wildlife conflict (Table 1).

Based on our thematic analysis, it appeared that wildlife species type, the development status of the nation where the study occurred, and whether the study was conducted on private lands versus in/adjacent to protected areas were relevant to what authors described as a human–wildlife conflict. For publications, the probability that the model including all direct effects (species type, the Human Development Index value for the nation where the study occurred, and whether or not the study was conducted in/adjacent to a protected area) was the best approximating model among those considered was >99.9% (wi > 0.999, Nagelkerke's R2= 0.560). This model was sufficiently dominant that considering competing models was unnecessary (Burnham & Anderson 2002:75–79). For conference presentations, the best approximating model (1) included species type and Human Development Index values (wi= 0.864, Nagelkerke's R2= 0.658). The next best approximating models—including (2) Human Development Index values (wi= 0.098, Nagelkerke's R2= 0.612) and (3) all direct effects—received considerably less support (wi= 0.030, Nagelkerke's R2= 0.686). Specifically, the evidence ratios (w1/wj) for models 2 and 3 were 8.8 and 28.4, indicating model 1 was approximately 9 and 28 times more likely to be the best approximating model as compared to models 2 and 3, respectively.

Human–wildlife conflict usage reflecting wildlife damaging human food resources was most prevalent for charismatic mega-herbivores (83%), but charismatic mega-carnivores (68%) and birds (68%) also were cited frequently in publications (Table 2). Meso-mammals were the only group primarily implicated in property damage (62%). Charismatic mega-carnivores were implicated more than twice as often in publications involving threats to human safety than any other species group. Patterns were similar for presentations at professional meetings, with the exception of meso-mammals being portrayed as threats to human safety more frequently (Table 2).

Overall, focusing on damage to food resources was most prevalent in research conducted in less developed nations, whereas threats to human safety and property damage were more common themes in research conducted in developed nations. Wildlife damaging human food resources was most prevalent in publications where research areas were in less developed nations (85%), and progressively less prevalent when in developing (78%) and developed (50%) nations (Table 2). Human safety and property damage were focal topics more often in publications focused on developed nations than those focused on less developed nations. Equating human–wildlife conflict with nonfood property damage was relatively common in research conducted in developed nations (27%), but rare for research in developing (1%) or less developed nations (4%). A similar pattern occurred in conference presentations (Table 2).

Human–wildlife conflict usage in publications reporting research conducted in or immediately adjacent to protected areas tended to reflect human–human conflict more commonly (8%) than similar research on private lands (2%; Table 2). Conversely, a property damage usage of human–wildlife conflict was more prevalent for research from private lands (18%) than for research from protected areas (3%).

Discussion

Actual conflict between wildlife and humans was rarely described in publications or presentations ostensibly addressing human–wildlife conflict (Table 1). Most cases referred to as human–wildlife conflict would be more accurately described as perceptions among people that wildlife threaten something they care about (e.g., health, safety, food, property; Conover 2002). Direct conflict of any type was rare in cases referred to as human–wildlife conflict, and when it did exist, it reflected human–human conflicts regarding how wildlife should be managed.

This study supports the assertion that human–human conflict is the primary type of conflict being labeled as human–wildlife conflict (Tables 1 and 2). Conflicts tended to occur among humans deciding how best to address potential wildlife threats to human property, health, safety, or development objectives. Wildlife in less developed countries has no more impetus to engage in conflict with humans over food than wildlife in developed nations. The driving factors relate to the human side of the equation (e.g., how people choose—or are pressured by other social institutions—to produce, store, and distribute food relative to wildlife populations). People living subsistence lifestyles are likely to experience greater impacts from, and thus be more concerned about, wildlife damaging their crops than are those who purchase food in plastic wrappers in supermarkets. Likewise, wildlife in more developed nations have no more reason to threaten human safety or nonfood property than do wildlife in less developed nations. Again, people's concerns about such threats stem from the human side of the equation. Finally, the predominant species type addressed in human–wildlife conflict studies reflects the trend of conflating human issues with human–wildlife conflict. The human–wildlife conflict studies targeting threats to human food production focused on charismatic megafauna, rather than the species doing most crop damage. Wildlife species of little interest to many conservation biologists (e.g., rodents, starlings) cause most of the $8.3 billion in wildlife damage expenses in urban areas and $4.5 billion in agricultural losses in the United States (Conover 2002; Pimentel 2002).

Researchers constructed the human–wildlife conflict terministic screen as an alternative to animal damage management and other ways of describing problematic interactions between humans and wildlife. Although the human–wildlife conflict terministic screen may have more cachet than animal damage management—and thus may appear likely to increase the stature of the subdiscipline, improve funding opportunities, and create a sense of urgency—this representation also constrains the way problems are defined and limits the array of potential solutions available. Solutions to animal damage problems, for example, differ from methods used to resolve human–human conflicts.

Animal damage can refer to anything wildlife do that any human dislikes (Conover 2002). When animal damage is portrayed as human–wildlife conflict, it may perpetuate the anthropomorphic view that animals possess humanlike consciousness, including values, interests, and intents—that they want what is ours, thus representing wild animals as human antagonists. For example, Dublin & Hoare (2004:271) state “human–elephant conflict … involves not only agricultural losses, but also a complex social dimension in the most affected sector, subsistence farming.” This terministic screen pits elephants against subsistence farmers, but the “complex social dimension” refers to human, not elephant, society. Neither party to this so-called human–wildlife conflict created the social context making elephants a threat to human food supplies (e.g., locally increased elephant numbers, encroachment between agricultural and conservation lands), and neither party holds the power to change those contexts.

Predictable but unintended consequences can accompany framing biodiversity conservation challenges as human–wildlife conflict. A human–wildlife conflict terministic screen places wildlife, entities that cannot represent themselves in the political sphere, in the role of combatants against people. If they accept the role of combatants, it makes sense for people to direct anger, frustration, and even attacks at wildlife rather than their human adversaries with potentially grave conservation consequences (Peterson et al. 2002; Brook et al. 2003). Conflicts labeled as human–wildlife conflict associated with the U.S. Endangered Species Act exemplify mislabeled human–human conflicts. Without the Act, there would be little animosity toward a host of rather benign wild species, such as the spotted owl (Strix occidentalis) or the Key deer (Odocoileus virginianus clavium), whose existence constrains human development projects only by virtue of their listing under the Act (Freudenburg et al. 1998; Peterson et al. 2002). Individuals from these species were hung in effigy by angry citizens in part because the human–wildlife conflict terministic screen conflated them with conscious human antagonists. These species cannot survive without protection, but humans, not the species themselves, are demanding the protection. Further, labeling human–human conflicts as human–wildlife conflicts may limit opportunities for conflict resolution by diverting attention from addressing conflicts within human political systems until they escalate to self-reinforcing levels that are much more difficult to resolve (Peterson et al. 2002).

Although most conservation researchers recognize that humans are part of, rather than apart from, nature, the human–wildlife conflict terministic screen threatens to attenuate that awareness. Labeling conflict between humans regarding biodiversity conservation and animal damage as human–wildlife conflict dichotomizes humans and nature, framing wildlife as something that threatens human existence, rather than contributing to human welfare. Yet shifting toward a terministic screen that directs attention toward the potential for coexistence between all species, while still recognizing the existence of multiple, sometimes contradictory needs, is difficult.

Given that the central concern identified in this study is the terministic screen used for environmental conflict, we suggest that rhetoric may also supply a corrective. Burke ([1937]1984; Peterson 1997) argues that human societies use both tragic and comic approaches to correct dysfunctional terministic screens. The more common, tragic, approach rejects and seeks to destroy the entire social reality engendered by the offending terministic screen. The comic corrective, however, recognizes terministic screens as products of fallible humans that simultaneously exceed and distort the intents of their creators. Because a comic corrective recognizes every perspective is an experiment, it should facilitate adaptive change. Recognizing that the internal contradictions associated with the label, human–wildlife conflict, have the unintended consequence of implicating wildlife in consciously antagonistic relations with humans opens the door to alternative ways of framing conflicts over biodiversity conservation. We suggest conservation researchers and practitioners utilize the comic corrective to pun the internal contradictions associated with human–wildlife conflict and experiment with developing a terministic screen from the label, human–wildlife coexistence.

Two recent examples highlight how conservation practitioners have used the comic corrective to begin shifting the human–wildlife conflict terministic screen. Francine Madden, executive director of the Human–Wildlife Conflict Collaboration, has helped develop a training program (http://www.humanwildlifeconflict.org/Training.htm) that focuses on improving participants’ ability to identify and address human–human conflicts that have been masked by the human–wildlife conflict label. This approach promotes a shift toward human–wildlife coexistence by highlighting the ambiguity inherent to relations among humans, and how those relationships influence interaction between humans and wildlife. Similarly, Mike Conover and Bruce Leopold, at the Jack H. Berryman Institute (A National Institute for Resolving Human–Wildlife Conflicts), provided constructive feedback during earlier drafts of this article, and played a role in the 2010 name change of the journal Human–Wildlife Conflicts to Human–Wildlife Interactions. The name change illustrates how a label can redirect attention to mutuality between humans and wildlife, while still addressing cases where humans and wildlife compete for limited resources.

Although nature is not inherently a rhetorical text, human actions and political institutions associated with it do function rhetorically. We argue that conservation scientists must attend to both verbal and material systems. Applying a comic corrective to the ways environmental conflict is rhetorically framed encourages us to reconsider our engagement in the political process; to remind anyone who will listen that we threaten our own species when we threaten the existence of others.

Editor: Dr. Sandra Jonker

Acknowledgments

We thank J. Garrow, anonymous reviewers, H. Bondell, and B. Collier for valuable constructive criticism. B. Leopold and M. Conover at the Jack H. Berryman Institute provided insight into the history of wildlife damage management and human–wildlife conflict. North Carolina State University, the Texas A&M University System, and the Swedish University of Agricultural Sciences supported this project. The opinions, findings, conclusions, or recommendations expressed in this document are those of the authors and should not be interpreted as representing the opinions or policies of the U.S. Government.

Ancillary

Article Information

DOI

10.1111/j.1755-263X.2010.00099.x

Format Available

Full text: HTML | PDF

©2010 Wiley Periodicals, Inc.

Request Permissions

Publication History

  • Issue online:
  • Version of record online:
  • Received: 29 July 2009; accepted 20 January 2010.

Keywords

  • Animal damage;
  • biodiversity conservation;
  • discourse;
  • environmental policy;
  • grounded theory;
  • human–wildlife conflict;
  • rhetoric;
  • social construction;
  • thematic analysis

References

  • Bagchi, S., Mishra C. (2006) Living with large carnivores: predation on livestock by the snow leopard (Uncia uncia). J Zool268, 217–224.
  • Bennett, E., Neiland A., Anang E. et al . (2001) Towards a better understanding of conflict management in tropical fisheries: evidence from Ghana, Bangladesh and the Caribbean. Marine Policy25, 365–376.
  • Brook, A., Zint M., De Young R. (2003) Landowners’ responses to an endangered species act listing and implications for encouraging conservation. Conserv Biol17, 1638–1649.
  • Burke K. (1966) Language as symbolic action: essays on life, literature, and method. University of California Press, Berkeley , CA .
  • Burke, K. (19371984) Attitudes toward history, 3rd Edition. University of California Press, Berkeley , CA, USA .
  • Burke, K. (19501969) A Rhetoric of Motives. University of California Press, Berkeley , CA .
  • Burnham, K.P., Anderson D.R. (2002) Model selection and multimodel inference: a practical information-theoretic approach. 2nd edn. Springer-Verlag, New York .
  • Chan, K.M.A., Pringle R.M., Ranganathan J. et al . (2007) When agendas collide: human welfare and biological conservation. Conserv Biol21, 59–68.
  • Conover, M.R. (2002) Resolving human–wildlife conflicts: the science of wildlife damage management. Lewis Publishers, Boca Raton , FL .
  • Corbin, J., Strauss A. (2008) Basics of qualitative research: techniques and procedures for developing grounded theory. 3rd Edition. Sage, Thousand Oaks , CA .
  • Czech, B., Krausman P.R., Borkhataria R. (1998) Social construction, political power and the allocation of benefits to endangered species. Conserv Biol12, 1103–1112.
  • Czech, B., Krausman P.R., Devers P.K. (2000) Economic associations among causes of species endangerment in the United States. Bioscience50, 593–601.
  • Dingwall, R. (2002) What makes conflict resolution possible?Negot J18, 321–326.
  • Dublin, H.T., Hoare R.E. (2004) Searching for solutions: the evolution of an integrated approach to understanding and mitigating human–elephant conflict in Africa. Hum Dimensions Wildl9, 271–278.
  • FitzGibbon, S.I., Jones D.N. (2006) A community-based wildlife survey: the knowledge and attitudes of residents of suburban Brisbane, with a focus on bandicoots. Wildl Res33, 233–241.
  • Forester, D.J., Machlis G.E. (1996) Modeling human factors that affect the loss of biodiversity. Conserv Biol10, 1253–1263.
  • Freudenburg, W.R., Wilson L.J., O’Leary D.J. (1998) Forty years of spotted owls? A longitudinal analysis of logging industry job losses. Sociol Perspect41, 1–26.
  • Goodstein, E.S. (1999) The trade-off myth: fact and fiction about jobs and the environment. Island Press, Washington , DC.
  • Graham, K., Beckerman A.P., Thirgood S. (2005) Human-predator-prey conflicts: ecological correlates, prey losses and patterns of management. Biol. Conserv122, 159–171.
  • Graham, M.D., Ochieng T. (2008) Uptake and performance of farm-based measures for reducing crop raiding by elephants Loxodonta africana among smallholder farms in Laikipia District, Kenya. Oryx42, 76–82.
  • Guyer, J., Richards P. (1996) The invention of biodiversity: social perspectives on the management of biological variety in Africa. Afr: J Int Afr Inst66, 1–13.
  • Herda-Rapp, A., Goedeke T.L. (2005) Mad about wildlife: looking at social conflict over wildlife. Brill, Boston , MA .
  • Jackson, R.M., Wangchuk R. (2004) A community-based approach to mitigating livestock depredation by snow leopards. Hum Dimensions Wildl9, 307–315.
  • Kaltenborn, B.P., Bjerke T., Nyahongo J. (2006) Living with problem animals: self-reported fear of potentially dangerous species in the Serengeti region, Tanzania. Hum Dimensions Wildl11, 397–409.
  • Kerr, J.T., Currie D.J. (1995) Effects of human activity on global extinction risk. Conserv Biol

0 Thoughts to “Research Papers On Human Elephant Conflict Pdf To Word

Leave a comment

L'indirizzo email non verrà pubblicato. I campi obbligatori sono contrassegnati *